Research Ethics

The Less Shocking Reality of Milgram’s Experiments Research Ethics
In conditions where the teacher was free to choose the shock levels, very few proceeded to the maximum voltage. (Photo: Isabelle/Ontario Science Centre: CC BY-NC-ND)

The Less Shocking Reality of Milgram’s Experiments

April 4, 2014 5106
Milgram's machine
When the teacher was free to choose the shock levels, few proceeded to the maximum voltage. (Photo: Isabelle/Ontario Science Centre/CC BY-NC-ND)

Chances are you’ve heard of Stanley Milgram’s obedience experiments. In 1961, Milgram recruited pairs of volunteers to take part in a “memory test.” One volunteer was given the job of teacher, the other, learner.

Each time the learner gave a wrong answer on a memory test the teacher was instructed to give the learner an electric shock and to increase the voltage with every error. What the teacher didn’t learn until later was that the learner and experimenter were actors and the machine was a prop.

In his first publication about the research, Milgram reported that a clear majority of participants inflicted what they were led to believe were painful if not lethal electric shocks to the learner. Most of us, he concluded, can be induced to torture someone else at the behest of an authority figure.

Milgram’s work appeared to say something profound about human nature. But there is no general agreement among psychologists about the meaning or implications of the research.

The Conversation logo_AU
This article by Nick Haslam and Gina Perry originally appeared at The Conversation, a Social Science Space partner site, under the title “Revisiting Milgram’s shocking obedience experiments.”

Did it, as Milgram argued, illuminate the Holocaust, whose executioners claimed they were merely following orders?

Did it reveal the capacity for evil lurking in our hearts?

Or did it simply demonstrate Milgram’s own blind-spots?

We decided to take a closer look at the statistical claims of Milgram’s research by revisiting his papers and data related to the obedience experiments in the archives at Yale University.

Milgram’s headline-grabbing conclusion that 62.5 percent of people obeyed instructions appeared to show most of us can be led to kill at an authority’s bidding. But this statistic came from his second, and most widely reported experiment, which involved just 40 people.

In fact, Milgram conducted 23 different kinds of experiments, each with a different scenario, script and actors. This patchwork of experimental conditions, each conducted with a sample of only 20 or 40 participants, yielded rates of obedience that varied from 0 percent to 92.5 percent, with an average of 43 percent. Contrary to received opinion, a majority of Milgram’s participants disobeyed.

In some conditions, the experimenter told the teacher to stop instead of continue. Others had two experimenters give conflicting orders. There were conditions with female teachers, or groups of teachers (using confederates again) who pressured the participant to obey or disobey.

In some conditions, the learner drew attention to a heart condition, in another he made no verbal responses at all, and in another he only agreed to take part if he could pull out when he wanted.

In several conditions the experimenter was physically distant from the teacher. In others the teacher was seated next to the learner in the same room. In one little-known condition, the learner was a friend or relative of the teacher.


From our partner
Pacific Standard …

The more conversations she had, and the more time she spent in the Yale archives, the more she came to believe that Milgram’s subjects had been doubly misled: first by the experimental conditions in the lab, and second by the notion that Milgram had proven anything at all.
Read more …


Finally, Milgram varied the setting in which the experiment took place. One condition took place in industrial town of Bridgeport, far from the ivied halls of Yale, the study’s home base.

Making systematic sense of those variations did not interest Milgram himself, but we have published a first attempt today in the journal PLOS ONE. Using his original data and backed up by research at Yale University’s archives, we synthesizsed data from 21 of his experimental conditions, involving 740 participants in all.

We classified the 21 conditions in terms of differing roles of the experimenter, teacher, learner and the relationships between the three.

Setting aside the broader problems with the experiment’s internal validity, evidence of participant skepticism, and the lack of standardized procedures, we aimed to tease out which of the many features of his different variations accounted for whether or not teachers went to the maximum voltage on the shock machine.

According to our analysis, the most powerful factor was whether or not the experimenter directed the teacher to administer the constantly rising shock levels. In conditions where the teacher was free to choose the shock levels, very few proceeded to the maximum voltage.

Obedience levels were significantly lower when there was dissent between the experimenters, when there was support for disobedience among the teachers and when the experimenter was absent from the room.
Interestingly, it was no higher when the experimenter was a more legitimate authority figure, or when the experiment was conducted in a more prestigious institutional setting.

Milgram ad

By implication, obedience is strongest when authority figures give concrete directives, present a united front and maintain close contact their subordinates. It is also strongest when subordinates lack collective support for resistance.

We found that the relationship between the learner and the teacher was equally important. Teachers were more likely to refuse to continue when the learner was physically close, when the learner was an intimate of the teacher and when the teacher had a direct link to the learner.

Obedience – in the Milgram paradigm at least – is not only a matter of the subordinate’s relationship to the authority figure. This is where most Milgram scholarship focuses, but it is only part of the story. Social relationships with people other than an authority figure are a powerful influence.

Ironically, because they were isolated in an experimental laboratory, Milgram’s subjects lacked the advantage available to others in the world outside when they are being coerced and pressured to obey. In the face of bullying, the best strategy is to find allies, form alliances, and stick together.The Conversation

Nick Haslam is a social psychologist at the University of Melbourne whose interests include prejudice, psychiatric classification and refugee mental health. His books include Psychology in the Bathroom, Introduction to Personality and Intelligence, Yearning to Breathe Free: Seeking Asylum in Australia and Introduction to the Taxometric Method. Gina Perry is is an Australian psychologist (and member of the Australian Psychological Society) and writer. Her co-production of the ABC Radio National documentary about the obedience experiments, ‘Beyond the Shock Machine,’ won the Silver World Medal for a history documentary in the 2009 New York Festivals radio awards.

View all posts by Nick Haslam and Gina Perry

Related Articles

Maintaining Anonymity In Double-Blind Peer Review During The Age of Artificial Intelligence
Research
August 23, 2023

Maintaining Anonymity In Double-Blind Peer Review During The Age of Artificial Intelligence

Read Now
Hype Terms In Research: Words Exaggerating Results Undermine Findings
Research Ethics
June 23, 2023

Hype Terms In Research: Words Exaggerating Results Undermine Findings

Read Now
Five Steps to Protect – and to Hear – Research Participants
Research Ethics
January 18, 2023

Five Steps to Protect – and to Hear – Research Participants

Read Now
We Developed a Tool to Make Responsible Research and Innovation Easier
Innovation
June 16, 2022

We Developed a Tool to Make Responsible Research and Innovation Easier

Read Now
COVID-19, Face Masks and Research Integrity

COVID-19, Face Masks and Research Integrity

Robert Dingwall asks if claims about the effectiveness of face masks in stopping COVID consistent with current standards of research integrity.

Read Now
To Study Zika, They Offered Their Kids. Then They Were Forgotten

To Study Zika, They Offered Their Kids. Then They Were Forgotten

“We feel diminished,” says Alessandra Hora dos Santos. “It’s like we were lab rats. They come in nicely, collect information, collect exams on the child, and in the end we don’t know of any results. It’s like we are being used without even knowing why that is being done.”

Read Now
NAS Creates Council to Address Research Integrity and Trust

NAS Creates Council to Address Research Integrity and Trust

A new blue-ribbon council convened by the United States’ National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine aims to tackle questions about nettlesome issues like conflict of interest, measuring impact and handling retractions.

Read Now
0 0 votes
Article Rating
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

1 Comment
Newest
Oldest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments