Opinion

A Former Student Reflects on How Daniel Kahneman Changed Our Understanding of Human Nature Opinion
Daniel Kahneman

A Former Student Reflects on How Daniel Kahneman Changed Our Understanding of Human Nature

April 12, 2024 2288

Daniel Kahneman’s passing at 90 years old has left a major gap in the field of behavioral science and in the wider intellectual community.

His scientific contributions, many made in collaboration with cognitive psychologist Amos Tversky, transformed the disciplines of psychology and economics. They also had outsize effects on philosophy, political science and many other disciplines.

I first met Danny in 1984, when I was a student. I moved to Vancouver, Canada to study in his lab at University of British Columbia, which he shared with psychologist Ann Triesman. Although Danny was not yet as famous as he was to become, his genius was widely recognized.

The Conversation logo
This article by Daniel Read originally appeared on The Conversation, a Social Science Space partner site, under the title “Daniel Kahneman changed how we think about human nature – the psychologist remembered by a former student.”

Three then or future Nobel prize winners visited the class. A young Richard Thaler spent a year with us and Thomas Schelling and Francis Crick came by. Despite this, Danny had time for everyone. I sometimes find myself giving the advice Danny gave me to my students and colleagues even today.

In 2002, Danny received the Nobel memorial prize in economic sciences. He then became perhaps the most influential and distinguished writer for the public in the behavioral sciences (rivaled by his close friend and collaborator Richard Thaler). Danny’s 2011 book Thinking, Fast and Slow is a classic of popular science writing.

In it he popularized and developed the idea of two systems of thought. One of the two systems is fast, efficient, confident and prone to error (system one), while the other is slow, resource-consuming, full of doubt and perhaps a little less prone to error (system two).

These systems work together. System one tells us the dessert is likely to be delicious and worth considering, while system two (might) step in to check the calorie count before we dig in.

In the academic world it would be hard to pinpoint Danny’s most important contributions, but prospect theory, set out in his 1979 paper with Tversky, is by far the most cited. The paper challenged the mainstream economic view that people were fundamentally “rational” even if prone to error.

Many scientists realised this could not be quite right. But Danny and Tversky showed that incorporating important psychological principles into economics could explain many puzzling observations about human nature.

Nudge psychology

The prospect theory paper also provided over a dozen experimental demonstrations for their claims. Teachers (like myself) still use these demonstrations in the classroom, so reliable are the results they reported.

A simple example: given a choice between £100 for sure and a 50/50 chance of gaining £200, most people will take the £100. But given a choice between losing £100 for sure and a 50/50 chance of losing £200, people will take the 50/50 chance. People are risk averse when it comes to gains, but risk-seeking about losses.

Prospect theory continues to generate new research, and is foundational to the science of “nudging.” Nudge theory is the notion that our behavior can be successfully influenced through “soft” interventions. One of its central principles is loss aversion, which means people are more sensitive to losses than gains of equivalent magnitude.

If you want people to bring their own mug to a coffee shop, charging them for a paper cup (a loss) is more effective than giving them a discount for bringing their own cup (a gain). The idea that behavior can be influenced by such biases is now part of the bread and butter of behavioral science.

Other prospect theory findings included the tendency for people to be “anchored” when they make quantitative judgments. For instance, if you put an offer on a house, the seller’s valuation will probably shift in the direction of your offer. Danny also described the planning fallacy, the tendency for projects to cost more and deliver less and take longer than expected.

Another fascinating line of work of Danny’s examined how a person’s feelings and judgments about events depend on their imagined alternatives to those events. One way imagination works is by “undoing” events. The easier they are to undo, the greater the effect they have on us. This rule was central to norm theory, his 1986 paper with Dale Miller.

One example of how undoing works: if a car crashes into the wall one meter from where we are standing, we believe we just missed being killed, and are relieved. But if tomorrow a car hits the exact spot where we are standing today, we probably give it less thought. Our imagination automatically creates a scenario in which we are standing one meter to the left now, but not one in which we are standing at the same place tomorrow.

The science of wellbeing

Danny also pioneered the analysis of well-being. Our reflections about an experience are more likely to be influenced by our interpretation of them than the positive or negative feelings we have during an experience.

The peak-end rule, for instance, is that the extent to which we like a past experience depends on the best or worst part of that experience, and how it ended. Danny showed that men receiving colonoscopies would report a better experience if the painful intrusion was a longer period of moderate pain, rather than one that ended earlier but with greater pain.

One way Danny stood apart from other researchers is that his work was driven by a desire not merely to contribute to a research field, but to create new fields. And then, if possible, to answer all the questions they pose. That is why his research, even published many decades ago, continues to act as the basis for new ideas and debates.

I last met Danny less than a year ago. I visited his home in New York for dinner. At one point during the evening Danny said that people were coming to see him to say goodbye.

His eyes were smiling and he held his hand up when he said it – a smile and gesture I have seen him use many times, a mixture of “I am not offended,” “you should not be offended by my bluntness” and “let me explain.” I did not want to face what he was saying but I understood. He was clear-eyed about the prospect of death. He was not afraid of death, he was not afraid of anything.

Daniel Read is a professor of behavioral economics at Warwick Business School He has held faculty positions at Leeds University Business School, London School of Economics (Reader) and Durham Business School (Professor), and visiting positions at INSEAD, Yale School of Management, and Rotterdam Business School.

View all posts by Daniel Read

Related Articles

‘Settler Colonialism’ and the Promised Land
International Debate
September 27, 2024

‘Settler Colonialism’ and the Promised Land

Read Now
Research Assessment, Scientometrics, and Qualitative v. Quantitative Measures
Impact
September 23, 2024

Research Assessment, Scientometrics, and Qualitative v. Quantitative Measures

Read Now
Let’s Return to Retractions Being Corrective, Not Punitive
Communication
July 15, 2024

Let’s Return to Retractions Being Corrective, Not Punitive

Read Now
The Long Arm of Criminality
Opinion
April 29, 2024

The Long Arm of Criminality

Read Now
Four Reasons to Stop Using the Word ‘Populism’

Four Reasons to Stop Using the Word ‘Populism’

Beyond poor academic practice, the careless use of the word ‘populism’ has also had a deleterious impact on wider public discourse, the authors argue.

Read Now
A Black History Addendum to the American Music Industry

A Black History Addendum to the American Music Industry

The new editor of the case study series on the music industry discusses the history of Black Americans in the recording industry.

Read Now
Book Review: A Memoir Highlighting Scientific Complexity

Book Review: A Memoir Highlighting Scientific Complexity

In this brief, crisply written memoir, “In a Flight of Starlings: The Wonders of Complex Systems,” Parisi takes the reader on a journey through his scientific life in the realm of complex, disordered systems, from fundamental particles to migratory birds. He argues that science’s struggle to understand and master the universe’s complexity, and especially to communicate it to an ever-more skeptical public, holds the key to humanity’s future well-being.

Read Now
0 0 votes
Article Rating
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

0 Comments
Newest
Oldest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments